Sabtu, 31 Oktober 2015

Chapter 1 and 2

CHAPTER 1 Introduction
Vocabulary is a key part of any language-teaching program. Nation (2001)
proposed four general goals that are important in a language classroom. These learning
goals concern: Language, which includes vocabulary; Ideas, which covers content and
subject matter as well as cultural knowledge; Skills; and finally Text or discourse (Nation
2001, p.1). Moreover, in learning a language, specifically for vocabulary goals, there are
three aspects to be looked at: the number of words in the language, the number of words
known by the native speakers, and the number of words needed by a learner to use the
language productively. The number of words in English and number known by English
native speakers are not the interest of the current study, which instead focuses on the third
aspect: the words needed to use English productively, specifically for EFL learners in
Indonesia.
The research literature in vocabulary learning in a second language (L2) has
revealed the importance of knowing a sufficient number of words to be able to function
in the language (Duin and Graves, 1987; Walker, Greenwood, Hart and Carta, 1994;
Nation, 2001; Read, 2004; Tschirner, 2004; Zimmerman, 2005). The development of
adequate vocabulary size is vitally important, and researchers have examined the use of
learning strategies as one means to foster the development of L2 vocabulary knowledge.
Strategies that have been proposed to help develop vocabulary learning include Memory
Strategies (MEM), Social Strategies (SOC), Cognitive Strategies (COG), Metacognitive
Strategies (MET) and Determination Strategies (DET) (Schmitt, 1997). Morin, 2003;
2
Chang, Wagner, Muse, Chow, and Shu; 2005; and Schiff and Calif, 2007) have also
suggested that use of morphological cues for inferring meaning can help L2 learning.
Although only a handful of studies have examined the role of morphological
awareness in L2 vocabulary development, the findings suggest that various aspects of
morphological awareness may be particularly useful for vocabulary building.
Morphological awareness is defined as the "awareness of and access to the meaning and
structure of morphemes in relation to word" (Chang et al., 2005, p. 417). For example,
Wysocki and Jenkins (1987) found that students were able to learn new words by
generalizing from those sharing a root morpheme. Pica (1988, as cited in Morin, 2003)
also states the importance of the study of interlanguage morphology and the belief that
"morpheme analysis can provide important insights into the sequences, processes, and
input relevant to second language acquisition" (Morin, 2003, p. 107).
This paper builds on this body of research by examining the relationship between
the English vocabulary size of EFL senior high school students in Indonesia and their
morphological awareness. The study presented here attempts to evaluate and possibly
extend findings from previous studies to the context of EFL learners in Indonesia. The
obstacle that lack of vocabulary knowledge presents for Indonesian EFL learners has
been noted (Nur, 2004). An ultimate aim of the study is to assess the potential value of
incorporating instruction in morphological awareness as part of EFL vocabulary
instruction in Indonesian settings, similar to the one examined here. There are three
research questions to be tackled in the current study:
1. What is the vocabulary size of the Indonesian senior high school students in the
study, and does this differ by area of study, Social Science and Natural Science?
3
2. What level of English morphological awareness do these learners possess?
3. Can measures of morphological awareness be systematically related to the
measure of English vocabulary size?
Following the introduction, the review of previous studies in vocabulary learning
and morphological awareness are discussed in Chapter 2. At the end of the discussion,
the three research questions are presented. The methodology used in the study is
described in Chapter 3, and the report on the results obtained is presented in Chapter 4.
Those results then are analysed in Chapter 5 and compared to findings from previous
studies. Finally, in Chapter 6, a brief conclusion of the current study, the pedagogical
implication, limitations, and suggestions for the further study are provided.
4 CHAPTER 2 Literature Review
All languages have a vocabulary, a set of words that is the basis for making and
understanding sentences (Miller, 1991). Therefore, "without some knowledge of that
vocabulary, neither language production nor language comprehension would be possible"
(Anglin, 1993, p.2). Laufer and Nation (1999) stated that vocabulary provides the
enabling knowledge, which is required to be successful in other areas of language
proficiency. Thus, this chapter begins with a review of studies on the importance of
vocabulary learning, followed by a discussion on morphological awareness as a
vocabulary learning strategy and its two aspects, Morpheme Identification Awareness
and Structural Morphological Awareness. Finally, it presents the three research questions
examined in the present study.
2.1. Vocabulary Learning
There have been many studies about the significance of vocabulary in language
learning. For example, Walker, Greenwood, Hart and Carta (1994) stated that early
vocabulary knowledge has been shown to be a strong predictor of school progress in the
first language (L1). They found that vocabulary knowledge was particularly important in
reading achievement. In addition, Tschirner (2004) states that vocabulary size has been
identified as one of the most important indicators of L2 reading proficiency and of
academic language skills in general. He discusses the relationship between the extent of
participants’ L2 English vocabulary and other background information such as length of
time spent in English language-speaking countries, number of English books read per
5
year, learning strategies, etc. In other studies, the size of students’ vocabulary has also
been found to closely correlate with L2 writing ability (Laufer and Nation, 1995; Laufer,
1998; Beglar and Hunt, 1999; Zimmerman, 2005). Furthermore, Duin and Graves (1987)
found that if students are given a related set of words (through an intensive vocabulary
instruction as a prewriting technique) before they write an essay in which the words
might be used, the quality of their writing improves.
In another study, Read (2004) found that L2 learners are typically aware of the
extent to which limitations in their vocabulary knowledge hinder their ability to
communicate effectively in the target language. This is because lexical items carry the
basic information load of the meanings they wish to comprehend and express. In other
words, the learners realize that knowing more vocabulary will have a direct effect on
their ability to use and further develop the L2 they are learning. Thus, vocabulary can
lead the learners to be more confident in using the language.
Words are the primary carriers of meaning, and it is widely recognized that there
is a strong relationship between the individual’s vocabulary size and his/her general
language proficiency (Vermeer, 2001; Zimmerman, 2005). Methods for learning
vocabulary, then, are an important part of language learning.
There is variety of ways in which a child learns vocabulary in the L1. These
include:
1) Experiential learning (Armbruster, Lehr, and Osborn, 2001). The authors claim
that a child learns most vocabulary through reading or listening to words
being used in context. In other words, children are able to develop vocabulary
through their experiences with the words.
6
2) Memorizing (Levin, Levin, Glasman and Nordwall, 1992). These authors
believe that students learn new words by memorizing. If students are able to
connect words to a familiar image or visualization, they are more likely to be
able to remember, retrieve, and use the words in sentences.
3) Using words repeatedly, namely, the students are given practice (Long and
Rule, 2004). The learners are provided with worksheets to practice words that
have already been introduced.
Some of the viewpoints discussed above are also adopted for vocabulary learning in the
L2 teaching context. Additionally, Anglin (1993), referring to some previous studies,
proposed three approaches in the research literature to the development of vocabulary
knowledge:
1) Direct instruction of vocabulary in school (McKeown, Beck, Omalson, and
Perfetti, 1983)
2) Learning words and their meanings from context, especially during reading
activities (Miller, 1991; Nagy and Anderson, 1984). In addition, Zimmerman
(2005) emphasizes that the primary method for acquiring new vocabulary
(breadth) and deepen understanding for existing vocabulary (depth) is through
extensive reading. Furthermore, Krashen, (1985, 1989, as cited in Morin,
2003) believes that reading is the most efficient way to learn vocabulary
naturally.
3) Applying morphological knowledge to infer the meanings of words (Nagy and
Anderson, 1984; Wysocki and Jenkins, 1987).
7
The third approach is the focus of this thesis. In particular, it will consider the
individual learners’ application of morphological knowledge as a vocabulary learning
strategy. Learning strategies can play an important role in development because they
encourage the learner’s active involvement in the learning process. Vocabulary
instruction is most effective when students are positively and actively involved in their
learning and they are allowed to use their own strategies to learn the vocabulary (Long
and Rule, 2004). Therefore, investigating instructional approaches to the use of
morpheme or root word families in teaching vocabulary, Long and Rule (2004) found
that the learners could develop their vocabulary better when vocabulary was taught
through concrete representations (i.e. using pictures and real objects) and morphological
analyses rather than more traditional class instruction methods (e.g. simply writing words
down, students note taking, no morphological analysis).
The use of morphological knowledge as a potential strategy for vocabulary
learning was the focus of the following studies. Anglin (1993) found that the students
could analyze the morphological structure of complex words which they have not
actually learned before to figure out the meanings. Morin (2003) proposed the strategy of
using morphological knowledge to infer word meanings, and with it, the need to develop
morphological awareness in the L2. She characterizes morphological awareness as the
ability to reflect on and manipulate morphemes and word formation rules in a language
Morin (2003). Similarly, Chang et al. (2005) define morphological awareness as "the
awareness of and access to the meaning and structure of morphemes" (the smallest units
of meaning in a language) in relation to words. They quote Carlisle (1995, p. 194), who
defines morphological awareness as "children’s conscious awareness of the morphemic
8
structure of words and their ability to reflect on and manipulate that structure". The focus
is on children’s abilities to distinguish and manipulate morphemes at the word level, so
that children’s knowledge of both inflections and derivations in language are
simultaneously considered.
English morphology involves knowledge of both inflectional and derivational
processes, and each makes a distinctive contribution to language learning and use.
Fromkin, Blair and Collins (1999) define inflectional morphology as changes in the form
of a word according to its grammatical function, for example, talk becomes talked to
indicate activity in the past time. On the other hand, derivational morphology concerns
changes of a word to give additional meaning to the original word (e.g. sufficient
becomes insufficient) and may be in a different grammatical class from the underived
word as well (e.g. beauty, a noun, becomes beautiful, an adjective).
Knowledge of inflectional morphology plays a key role in grammatical accuracy,
while knowledge of derivational morphology plays a role in the development of
vocabulary knowledge. The role of learner knowledge of both inflectional and
derivational morphology in the development of L2 vocabulary is the focus of the present
study.
The study examines the relationship between morphological awareness and
vocabulary knowledge for the EFL Indonesian learners, with a focus on possible
implications this relationship might have for incorporating morphological awareness as a
part of vocabulary instruction in the L2 classroom. Morphological awareness will be
discussed in the next section.
9
2.2. Morphological Awareness as a Vocabulary Learning Strategy
As noted above, morphological awareness refers to the awareness of and access to
the meaning and structure of morphemes that are part of or related to the word. It
includes knowledge of derivational morphology such as prefixes (e.g., the un- in
undisciplined to indicate the antonym of the original, disciplined), suffixes (e.g., the -ion
in graduation changes the part of speech of the base word –graduate is a verb whereas
graduation is a noun), and compounding (e.g., cowboy to create new word combining the
two root morphemes: cow and boy). On the other hand, knowledge of inflectional
morphology focuses primarily on indicating grammatical changes in words (e.g., the s in
dogs to indicate the plural form of the base or the -ed in acted to refer to the action in the
past time).
Kuo and Anderson (2006) argue that morphological awareness in L1 English
becomes an increasingly important predictor of reading ability, as children grow older
because this awareness contributes to the decoding of morphologically complex words
and it is therefore assumed to contribute to the development of reading comprehension.
They also suggested that morphological awareness is intertwined with other aspects of
metalinguistic awareness and linguistic competence, especially phonological awareness,
syntactic awareness, and vocabulary knowledge.
Schiff and Calif (2007) compared previous studies that investigated the
relationship between phonology and reading, and morphology and reading. They found
that the relationship between phonology and reading development in English (as an L1) is
well-documented (Nagy and Anderson, 1998), but the parallel relationship between
morphological awareness and reading skill has been less studied (Singson, Mahony, and
10
Mann, 2000). Even fewer studies have dealt with vocabulary learning and morphology or
morphological awareness, but the small corpus of existing research suggests a strong link
between morphological awareness and vocabulary learning. Prince (2007) reports a study
done by Nonie Lesaux (in press), that shows that a learner who understands how words
are formed, by combining prefixes, suffixes, and roots, tends to have larger vocabularies
and better reading comprehension. The main concern for this present study is to relate
morphological knowledge to vocabulary learning in the L2.
The type of morphological knowledge, namely derivations and inflections, will
also have an effect on vocabulary learning (Anglin, 1993). For example, previous
research suggests that derived words might be acquired somewhat later than inflected and
compound words (Wysocki and Jenkins, 1987) and that, morphologically speaking,
words that are more complex generally will be acquired later than simpler words (Clark
and Berman, 1987).
Nunes, Bryant, and Bindman (2006) reported that the English L1 children took
several years to learn to use the -ed ending systematically to denote the past of regular
verbs: even at the age of 10 years many children still made mistakes with this
morphologically based rule. The authors concluded that awareness of morphology
influences children’s knowledge of when and when not to use the morpheme -ed.
Moreover, Fowler and Liberman (1995) assessed children’s knowledge of the connection
between a base and a derived form, and proposed three measures of progress in literacy
(word recognition, pseudo-word decoding, and spelling). They observed significant
correlations between all three measures of literacy and performance in the morphological
awareness tasks even after controlling for age and vocabulary. Of interest in the present
11
study is whether EFL students in Indonesia (after five years of learning English at
schools) acquire the aspects of morphological knowledge in a manner similar to that
reported in studies in an L1 context.
Chang et al. (2005) note that there are differences in languages as to the relative
importance of derivational and inflectional morphemes. For example, inflectional
morphology is obviously important in English or Finnish, but is relatively unimportant in
Chinese. In contrast, lexical compounding is far more common in Chinese than it is in
English. Bertram, Laine and Virkkala (2000) examined the role that morphology plays in
vocabulary acquisition in L1 Finnish. Systematically, they investigated the role that affix
frequency and productivity might play in the development of the children’s knowledge of
words. The results showed that the Finnish elementary school children benefit
significantly from utilizing morphology in determining word meanings.
In contrast to the research done on morphological awareness in the L1, there have
been only a few studies that have focused on morphological awareness in the L2. In order
to investigate the role of morphological awareness in developing vocabulary for L2
learners, Morin (2003) studied Spanish classes to examine the acquisition of derivational
morphology - the use of suffixes that can change the part of speech and cause variations
in meaning - by native English-speaking learners of Spanish. In this study the main
questions were: (1) Do beginning L2 learners who focus on Spanish derivational
morphology learn more vocabulary than learners who do not, (2) can they apply
morphological knowledge receptively and productively, and (3) does their success
depend on their L2 proficiency level. The results indicate that the strategy for building
vocabulary by consistently focusing on Spanish derivational morphology may yield
12
immediate benefits in the area of production, at least among one experimental group, the
second-semester learners, who were introduced to Spanish morphology. There is also a
suggestion that, for second-semester learners, there may be benefits or effects of such
knowledge of derivational morphology with respect to their receptive morphological
knowledge. In addition, the second-semester experimental group demonstrated a
significantly greater knowledge of productive Spanish derivational morphology than any
of the other groups studied. In her conclusion, Morin (2003) emphasized that the results
of her study could not make specific claims to all L2 learners generally. However, it does
indicate a positive trend in the effectiveness of morphological knowledge as a tool for
building vocabulary knowledge.
Morin (2003), Schiff and Calif (2007) both cite Koda (2000) who underscores the
effect that L1 knowledge can have on L2 morphological awareness. Schiff and Calif
(2007) examined the effect of phonology and morphology awareness in Hebrew (L1) on
L2 English development. Their findings revealed that the more similar the language
features, the more positive the cross-linguistic influence in terms of learning outcomes,
whereas for languages that are less similar, a less positive influence was shown.
In the current study, an attempt is made to investigate if the results of the previous
studies hold when the L1 is Bahasa Indonesia. Tala (2003) stated that Bahasa Indonesia
morphology can be considered simpler than English because it does not mark tenses,
gender or plural forms. However, Sneddon (1996) found that two groups of verbs in
Bahasa Indonesia primarily occur with form of affixation - prefixes and/or suffixes. In
addition, the affixation rules for verb bases in Bahasa Indonesia can be unpredictable,
with total mastery predominantly evident in native speakers only (Sneddon, 1996).
13
Compared to the learners’ Bahasa Indonesia, English is more complicated
morphologically. Nunens and Bryant (2004) show that there are many words in English
whose spelling cannot be predicted from phonology, but are entirely regular if analyzed
into morphemes. The word madness, for example, ends with a double s; this is entirely
predictable from the fixed spelling of the suffix -ness but not from phonology. Similarly,
the word musician would be considered highly irregular if it was analyzed in terms of
letter-sound correspondences, but its spelling is completely regular considering it was
formed by music and the suffix -ian, a morpheme to indicate ‘doer’ or ‘person who xs’
(where x refers to the noun the suffix attaches to). They conclude that an awareness of
morphology should benefit the development of children's vocabulary. Thus, for L1
learners, knowledge of English morphology makes a significant contribution to the
vocabulary size and other language skills. This present study is then aimed to investigate
if such knowledge makes a significant contribution to English vocabulary learning for
EFL students in Indonesia.
2.3 Measuring English Morphological Awareness
Anglin (1993) identifies five different morphological word types in English. The
five types are root words (e.g., short, closet), inflected words (e.g., smoking, reports),
derived words (e.g, shortish, treelet), literal compounds (e.g., sunburn, birthday), and
opaque, idiomatic compounds or lexical idioms, which are then called simply ‘idioms’
(e.g., mouse tail, "a plant of the crowfoot family"; pink lady, "a cocktail").
In this present study, four of the morphological word types (root words, inflected
words, derived words and literal compounds) were used to investigate the two types of
14
morphological awareness: Morpheme Identification Awareness and Morphological
Structural Awareness. The Morpheme Identification task tests the participants’
knowledge of root words and use of morphemes to guess meaning, whereas the
Morphological Structure task assesses the ability to create literal compounds, inflected,
and derived words. Further discussion on these two measures of morphological
awareness is provided in the next sections.
2.3.1. Morpheme Identification Awareness
Chang, et al. (2005) define awareness of Morpheme Identification as the ability to
distinguish different meanings across homophones, for example by understanding that
flower in flowerpot is represented by a plant with petals as opposed to a sack of white
powder (flour). The authors believe that this aspect of morphological awareness might
help language learners to distinguish among meanings of syllables with identical sounds,
facilitating language analysis and vocabulary growth. In this case, morphological
awareness involves understanding that different meanings can simultaneously be
attributable to phonologically identical words. For example, in the Morpheme
Identification task used here, the participant is shown a picture of the sun and a picture of
a male child and then given the word grandson. The participant is then asked to choose
which picture correctly reflects the meaning of the word. This is an adaptation of the test
used in study on young children by Chang et al. (2005). See appendix B, part 1 in Section
B.
15
2.3.2. Morphological Structure Awareness
The other type of morphological knowledge measured in this study is the
awareness of Morphological Structure. This Morphological Structure requires learners to
make use of linguistic knowledge to derive new meanings. Skill in manipulating
language, variously referred to as generativity, creativity, or productivity of language,
may be important in learning new meanings within one’s language (Chang et al., 2005, p.
421). For example, in the Morphological Structure Test used here the participant is given
a single sentence scenario and a prompt to make a novel compound word, as in:
There’s a paper that is white in color, we call that white paper.
There’s a paper that is red in color, what do we call it? ——————– (red paper)
The participants’ knowledge of inflectional morphology is also assessed in the
test by providing a context and then requiring the grammatically appropriate novel
response.
John is stotting. Yesterday he did this. What did he do yesterday?
Yesterday, he ________________________
Of interest here is whether the knowledge required to complete these Morpheme
Identification and Morphological Structure Awareness tasks relate to L2 vocabulary
knowledge. Chang et al. (2005) believe this is important because it demonstrates that
there are two different aspects of morphological awareness and that both of these might
be important in fostering vocabulary acquisition.
16
2.4. Research Questions
A major challenge facing EFL learners like those in the Indonesian context is the
development of a vocabulary of the size that would permit them to function adequately in
many English language situations (Nurweni and Read, 1999, as cited in Nur, 2004).
Through a research study conducted in an Indonesian university to estimate the English
vocabulary knowledge of first-year students, the authors found that only a small
proportion of the students came close to the threshold, in terms of breadth and depth of
their vocabulary knowledge. This study was the only one found that addressed the issue
of the English vocabulary size and proficiency of Indonesian EFL students. In addition,
when considering the link between morphological awareness and vocabulary, there have
only been a limited number of studies done on languages such as Finnish, Spanish and
Hebrew, see discussion above (Section 2.2.) Therefore, more research is needed to
provide a stronger empirical basis for our understanding of the issue. Motivated by earlier
studies, this research will investigate the importance of morphological awareness in
learning and teaching English vocabulary in Indonesian schools.
This study specifically aims to consider theoretical and practical aspects of
measurement of morphological awareness and how they relate to the learners’ EFL
vocabulary knowledge. Research in the L1 has shown that understanding and mastery of
morphology have been shown to be effective for building vocabulary. However, it is still
largely unknown whether strategies for vocabulary building that prove fruitful for L1
learners also produce significant gains in learners of an L2.
The primary goal of this study is to investigate whether morphological awareness
can be related to the vocabulary size of EFL Indonesian senior high school students. First,
17
the English vocabulary size of the Indonesian senior high school students in Social
Science and Natural Science programs will be measured. Then a measure of English
morphological awareness for these learners will be obtained. Of interest will be whether
there is a difference between these two groups in both vocabulary size and morphological
awareness. Finally, the link between vocabulary size and morphological awareness will
be assessed, with possible implications for morphological awareness as a predictor of
vocabulary learning.

Tidak ada komentar: