Rabu, 21 Oktober 2015

Conclusion

Conclusion 
The results of this study point to several things. Firstly, the data support the notion that words can be learned incidentally from context. However, these data suggest that few new words appear to be learned from this type of reading, and half of those that are learned are soon lost. Secondly, the test type affects the gain scores that are shown from the reading. Therefore, researchers should be particularly cautious about selecting multiple-choice tests to validate the learning of vocabulary. Thirdly, previous research that used a multiple-choice test format rather than a translation test most probably has overstated learning gains. Fourthly, those studies that did not have vocabulary retention data almost certainly will have overstated natural learning too. Thus, the results here suggest that studies that had both these elements in their design, appear to have substantially overstated their natural vocabulary gains. This should be borne in mind when interpreting their results. However, we have to be cautious when saying that very little vocabulary can be learned from the reading. This study only looked at the learning of new words from the reading. This study did not attempt to study a myriad of other forms of word knowledge which include lexical access speed gains; the noticing of collocations, colligations or patterns within text; the recognition of new word forms yet to be learned; an increase in the ability to guess from context; a confirmation that a previously guessed word's meaning is probably correct; recognition of new word associations; the raising of the ability to recognize discourse and text structure; an increase in the ability to 'chunk' text; the development of saccadic eye movements and so on, and so on. The jury is still out on these. Research into what effect reading in a foreign language has on these elements of the reading puzzle is welcomed. However, it is our contention that ultimately learners do not learn a lot ofnew words from graded reading, but in fact graded reading helps to
RFL 15.2 – At what rate do learners learn and retain new vocabulary from reading a graded reader?
http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl
154 deepen and consolidate already known language. The data presented here should not be interpreted as a case against the need for graded reading. Also it must not be forgotten that the data have been gathered from the reading of only one graded reader. Clearly, graded readers are not supposed to be used as a one-off learning experience with the vague hope that some new words or language will be learned. Unfortunately, all too often this is the case as graded readers are subjected to the 'supplementary' shelf of the teacher's armory. This research points ever more clearly to the need for repeated and consistent exposure to graded readers if words are going to be acquired and especially if the aim is to learn new words. However, there are clear implications for the recycling of vocabulary for the series editors of graded readers. These data suggest three possible courses that a series editor could take for the rate of repetition within a single graded reader, or within a level, or series. Firstly, a graded reader series editor could largely ignore the vocabulary requirements (in terms of volume of text, rate of repetition, and the chance of learning) in favour of letting the story control the vocabulary. A second option would be relevant if the series editor wishes to aid the learning of
new vocabulary. This could be done by identifying certain target words which may need to be highlighted and repeated over ten to twenty times within a book, or certainly within a level. This seems like an unnecessarily hard constraint on both authors and series editors of graded readers as the naturalness of a book may be distorted by vocabulary requirements. However, at least from the perspective of learning new words from graded reading, considerable care should be taken to ensure that the headwords chosen for a particular reader level are be recycled throughout the level, even if not within a particular title. It is not necessary to ensure that all the headwords appear a minimum number of times with each title, but it seems to be necessary within each level. Not doing so will mean that there is a high chance that new words will be forgotten. At the early levels of graded reader series, this is rather easier to do because the word lists are rather small and self-contained. However, at the later levels, it becomes more difficult as the range of words an author may wish to incorporate into a graded reader will be much wider. The third choice is for the series editor to not be overly concerned with presenting new vocabulary but provide a rich input of already known vocabulary in various contexts and with a variety of collocations and colligations. The tension among these three choices will remain with us in perpetuity but each has its own implications for the syllabus design of the graded readers. While this study has given us a few more insights into what kinds of vocabulary are learned from reading, and the rate at which words need to be met in order to learn them, there are still several unanswered questions. Firstly, we are still not clear whether increasing the number of occurrences of target items will lead to higher acquisition rates. If the subjects had met the target items say 25 or 30 times we can presume that more of them would have been learned, but this is not clear. Another unanswered question concerns whether it is due to the nature of graded reading itself (where the focus is on understanding the message rather than on the learning of new vocabulary) that certain words cannot be learned easily in this way. It may be that the type of cognitive effort expended depending on whether the subject is focused on word learning or on the message may be part of the explanation. This fruitful research area may also investigate whether certain types of words are best learned from reading than others, or whether they are best learned out of context. Other questions relate to how much vocabulary is learned by
RFL 15.2 – At what rate do learners learn and retain new vocabulary from reading a graded reader?
http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl
155 reading, say, all the titles of one level of a graded reader series, in order to determine just how many titles need to be read to master the vocabulary at that level. In conclusion, the results of this study seem to support Nation and Wang's (1999) research that recommends a high volume of reading (a book a week at the learner's reading level), or more. If this amount of reading were done, the rather disappointing forgetting rate evident from reading only one book would be reduced to some degree. The data also support Nation and Wang's contention that graded readers might be best used for recycling already known vocabulary than for introducing new words. This is because the results of this and other studies suggest that few
new words seem to be learned from graded reading. As has been mentioned elsewhere, vocabulary growth is not the main aim of graded or extensive reading (e.g., Day and Bamford, 1998, 2002; Waring, 1997; Waring and Takahashi, 2000; Prowse, 2002). Teachers and learners alike would be best advised to be aware of this and not to expect too many new words to be learned from their graded readers. However, learners should be encouraged to read them for the other informational and enjoyable aspects of reading in a foreign language, as well as the many language learning and affective benefits they offer.
Acknowledgement We thank Nick Bullard of Oxford University Press for his kind permission to allow us to use a digital version of A Little Princess.
References Anderson, R. C. and Freebody, P. (1981). Reading comprehension and the assessment and acquisition of word knowledge. In J. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching (pp. 77-117). Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association. Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory. London: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates. Bensoussan, M. & Laufer, B. (1984). Lexical guessing in context in EFL reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 7, 15-32. Burnett, F. H. retold by J. Bassett. (1991). A little princess. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Coady, J. (1997). L2 vocabulary acquisition through extensive reading. In J. Coady and T. Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition: A rationale for pedagogy (pp. 225-237). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Day, R. & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading in the second language classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Day, R & Bamford, J. (2002). Top ten principles for teaching extensive reading. Reading in a
Foreign Language,14(2), 136-141. http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl/October2002/day/day.html
RFL 15.2 – At what rate do learners learn and retain new vocabulary from reading a graded reader?
http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl
156 Day, R., Omura, C., & Hiramatsu, M. (1991). Incidental EFL vocabulary learning and reading.
Reading in a Foreign Language, 7(2), 541-551. Dupuy, B. & Krashen, S. (1993). Incidental vocabulary acquisition in French as a foreign language. Applied Language Learning, 4, 55-64. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1913). Über das Gedächtnis. Leipzig: Dunker. (Translation by H. Ruyer and C. Bussenius, 1913, Memory. New York: Teacher's College, Columbia University.) Grabe, W. & Stoller, F. (1997). Reading and vocabulary development in a second language: A case study. In J. Coady & T. Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition: A
rationale for pedagogy (pp. 98-122). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hayashi, K. (1999). Reading strategies and extensive reading in EFL classes. RELC Journal 30, 114-132. Horst, M., Cobb, T., & Meara, P. (1998). Beyond a Clockwork Orange: Acquiring second language vocabulary through reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 11(2), 207-223. Hu, M. & Nation, P. (2000). Unknown vocabulary density and reading comprehension.
Reading in a Foreign Language, 13(1), 403-430. Hulstijn, J. (1992). Retention of inferred and given word meanings: Experiments in incidental vocabulary learning. In P. Arnaud & H. Bejoint (Eds.), Vocabulary and applied
linguistics (pp. 113-125). London: Macmillan. Krashen, S. (1993). The power of reading: Insights from the research. Englewood, Co.: Libraries Unlimited. Laufer, B. & Sim, D. (1985). An attempt to measure the threshold of competence for reading comprehension. Foreign Language Annals, 18(5), 405-411. Liu Na & Nation, P. (1985). Factors affecting guessing vocabulary in context. RELC Journal, 16, 33-42. Mason, B. & Krashen, S. (1997). Extensive reading in English as a foreign language. System, 25, 91-102. Meara, P. & Buxton, B. (1987). An alternative to multiple choice vocabulary tests. Language
Testing, 4, 142-151. Nagy, W., Herman, P., & Anderson, R. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading Research
Quarterly, 20, 233-253. Nagy, W., Anderson, R., & Herman, P. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal, 24 263-282.
RFL 15.2 – At what rate do learners learn and retain new vocabulary from reading a graded reader?
http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl
157 Nation, P. (1999). Learning vocabulary in another language. English Language Institute Occasional Publication 19. Victoria University Wellington, New Zealand. Nation, P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Nation, P. & Wang, M. (1999). Graded readers and vocabulary. Reading in a Foreign Language, 12, 355-380. Pitts, M., White, H., & Krashen, S. (1989). Acquiring second language vocabulary through reading: A replication of the Clockwork Orange study using second language acquirers.
Reading in a Foreign Language, 5(2), 271-275. Prowse, P. (2002). Top ten principles for teaching extensive reading -- a response. Reading in a
Foreign Language, 14(2), 142-145. http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/rfl/October2002/discussion/prowse.html Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129-158. Shu, H., Anderson, R. & Zhang, Z. (1995). Incidental learning of word meanings while reading: A Chinese and American cross-cultural study. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 76-95. Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89-105). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Waring, R. (Ed.). (1997). Special edition on extensive reading. The Language Teacher, 21. Waring, R. (1999). Tasks for assessing receptive and productive second language vocabulary. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Wales, Swansea. http://www1.harenet.ne.jp/~waring/papers/phd/title.html Waring, R. & Takahashi, S. (2000). The Oxford University Press guide to the 'why and 'how' of
using graded readers. Tokyo: Oxford University Press. Waring, R. (in preparation). Extensive reading in second languages: A critique of the research. Draft available at http://www1.harenet.ne.jp/~waring/papers/assesser.html Wesche, M. & Paribakht, T. (1996). Assessing vocabulary knowledge: Depth vs. breadth.
Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 13-40. Yamazaki, A. (1996). Vocabulary acquisition through extensive reading. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Temple University, Japan.

Tidak ada komentar: